- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Gene expression analysis reveals the tipping points during infant brain development for human and chimpanzee
BMC Genomics volume 21, Article number: 74 (2020)
Abstract
Background
Postpartum developmental delay has been proposed as an important phenotype of human evolution which contributes to many human-specific features including the increase in brain size and the advanced human-specific cognitive traits. However, the biological processes and molecular functions underlying early brain development still remain poorly understood, especially in human and primates.
Results
In this paper, we comparatively and extensively studied dorsolarteral prefrontal cortex expression data in human and chimpanzee to investigate the critical processes or biological events during early brain development at a molecular level. By using the dynamic network biomarker (DNB) model, we found that there are tipping points around 3 months and 1 month, which are crucial periods in infant human and chimpanzee brain development, respectively. In particular, we shown that the human postnatal development and the corresponding expression changes are delayed 3 times relative to chimpanzee, and we also revealed that many common biological processes are highly involved in those critical periods for both human and chimpanzee, e.g., physiological system development functions, nervous system development, organismal development and tissue morphology. These findings support that the maximal rates of brain growth will be in those two critical periods for respective human and primates. In addition, different from chimpanzee, our analytic results also showed that human can further develop a number of advanced behavior functions around this tipping point (around 3 months), such as the ability of learning and memory.
Conclusion
This work not only provides biological insights into primate brain development at a molecular level but also opens a new way to study the criticality of nonlinear biological processes based on the observed omics data.
Background
The primate brain development was traditionally studied by investigating the conserved biological processes and functions across mammals [1]. Genetic changes resulting in protein changes are probably too few to account for the great phenotype differences between humans and chimpanzees, prompting the hypothesis that changes in gene expression are likely to drive major phenotypic differences between humans and other primates [2]. Human and chimpanzee also show a number of significant differences in morphology and numerous cognitive traits during development tendency [3,4,5,6,7]. Some detailed comparisons related to gene expression of human and chimpanzee have identified human accelerated regions [8, 9] or conserved noncoding sequences [10]. The individual ontogenesis of human and chimpanzee have mainly been compared in terms of skeletal morphology but there are few studies conducted at a molecular level. Results from these comparisons demonstrate that some human features may indeed be explained by neoteny, e.g. small jaws [11]. In human brain growth, developmental retardation or neoteny has been identified and also the brain developmental changes have been delayed comparing to other primate species. Besides, there is a well-known evidence that human’s infant brain develops sharply in the first 3 months in terms of morphology. The infant structural growth rate changes approximately from 1%/d to 0.4%/d at the end of 3 months [12]. Generally, the brain-growth rate of infant chimpanzee is three times as much as that of human.
The transcriptome is dramatically remodeled during postnatal brain development [13]. However, the biological processes and biological functions during early brain development in human and primates have not yet been extensively studied so far. In particular, the brain development can be considered as a nonlinear biological process, which involves the gradual change and then drastic transition near the tipping point. Thus, detecting the tipping point and further revealing the related molecular functions as well as gene regulations are important to understand the brain development as a nonlinear dynamical process at a molecular level. In this paper, we investigate the critical processes or events during early brain development at a molecular level by extensively studying dorsolarteral prefrontal cortex expression data in human and chimpanzee. For the first time, we identified 3 months for human and 1 month for chimpanzee as their respective tipping points during their infant brain development, based on Dynamic Network Biomarker (DNB) theory [14, 15]. In addition, we found three times difference in terms of brain growth rate between human and chimpanzee due to human slow postnatal development. We also reveal many common biological processes involved in those key periods for both human and chimpanzee, e.g., physiological system development functions, nervous system development, organismal development and tissue morphology, etc. Actually, many published works have reported that the maximal rates of brain growth are 3 and 1 month for human and chimpanzee respectively, which is consistent with our analysis. Our analytic results also show that from around 3 months (tipping point) to later, human rather than chimpanzee can further develop a number of advanced behavior functions, e.g. the ability of learning and memory. This work not only provides biological insights into the brain development from a system viewpoint but also opens a new way to study the criticality of nonlinear biological processes based on DNB theory.
Results
Expression pattern analysis of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex expression data reveals smooth changes with drastic transitions during brain development
We first evaluate the general expression pattern in the dorsolarteral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) of three species, human, chimpanzee and macaque. We cluster these samples according to their expression levels by hierarchical clustering analysis. The samples are well separated into three clusters according to their species. Relative to macaque samples, chimpanzee is close to human in the resulting clusters in terms of the distance (Additional file 1). Meanwhile, human and chimpanzee also can be clustered into two different groups (Fig. 1a). Therefore, human and chimpanzee brains are considered very close but still different. Clearly, there are smooth changes with drastic transitions during the period. In this work, we focus on the analyses of infant brain development of human and chimpanzee, i.e. focusing on the early / first year life stages of humans and chimpanzees. In the principal component analysis (PCA) results of the data, we can also find that two groups, i.e. human and chimpanzee, are significantly separated (Fig. 1b). Next, we estimate when these gene expression changes take place during human and chimpanzee brain development and what the differences are between human and chimpanzee among the whole gene expressions. We used Multi-dimensional scaling (MDS) to evaluate the global changes of the whole samples in human and chimpanzee gene expressions relative to the individual’s age. We find that the most rapid changes or drastic transitions take place in the first year for both species. As shown in Fig. 1, the slope of the line for gene expressions is approximately 1 during the first year. More than 55% changes happened in the first year. Furthermore, the early trajectory of chimpanzee’s age-related gene expression changes is in close proximity to that of human (Fig. 1c).
Dynamic network biomarkers identifies the tipping points during the infant brain development
Recent research works show that there is a sudden change or a critical transition during time evolution of many dynamic systems, such as climate system [16, 17], ecosystem [18, 19], economics, global finance [20, 21] and biological system [14]. Such a change plays a critical role in the development of whole system, and there will be a tipping point just before such drastic change or transition of system states. Owing to the sudden change, the dynamical features of biological system is from a state to another state through a drastic transition or transformation (Fig. 2a). To detect the tipping points during dynamic processes, a mathematical model, DNB theory with three quantitative criteria [14] was developed and has been widely applied to investigate many diseases [14, 15, 22,23,24,25,26,27] and biological processes [24, 28, 29] based on the observed data. In this work, DNB is firstly applied to find whether or not there is a tipping point in the primate brain growth in the first year (infant) and what biological functions are developed (enriched) before and after such tipping point.
To identify specific tipping points during the first year brain development of human and chimpanzee, the gene expression data include 13 infant humans and 9 infant chimpanzees were used. According to the three criteria of DNB for detecting tipping points [14], we analyzed all 17,429 genes. The composite index of the DNB suggests that human’s DLPFC gene expression has a significant change around 3 months (Fig. 2b) and around 1 month for chimpanzee (Fig. 2c). Additional file 2 also shows that 3 and 1 month are the tipping points of the infant human and chimpanzee respectively. By using the DNB model, we got 371 dynamical network biomarker genes (DNBs) (Fig. 3a, and Additional file 3) of human and 132 DNBs (Fig. 3a, and Additional file 3) of chimpanzee. There is one gene (TNFAIP3) overlapping the two specie-specific DNBs (Fig. 3a and Additional file 3). Meanwhile, we obtained 341 significantly differentially expressed genes (DEGs) around 3 months for human (Fig. 3b, and Additional file 4) (ANOVA false discovery rate (FDR) < 0.01 and fold-change > 2); and similarly, we detected 396 significant DEGs for chimpanzee (Fig. 3b, and Additional file 4). There are 18 genes overlapping the two DEGs (Fig. 3b and Additional file 4). Previous reports of these overlapping genes relevant to the functions of brain, especially on brain development, are listed in Table 1.
According to the DNB theory, DNBs may have strongly functional impacts on biological processes at the tipping points. Next we examine whether or not there are significantly differential expression changes during the development of infant brain for DNBs. We find that the global expression tendency of DNBs is significantly stronger than other genes. Obviously, the relative growth rate of infant chimpanzee is much higher than human (Fig. 3c) especially in the first year. There is the similar tendency for the DEGs of the two groups (Fig. 3d). These results suggest that DNBs and DEGs may play important roles around the tipping points during the brain growth together. Therefore, by analyzing DNBs and DEGs, we have shown a rapidly brain development mechanism for the coordination of gene expression in infant chimpanzee relative to infant human.
Advanced behavior related functions develop for human at the tipping point in contrast to chimpanzee
We carried out analysis of both human’s DNBs and DEGs before and after 3 months with IPA (Ingenuity Pathway Analysis) [42]. The most significant physiological system development and function terms from Disease and Function analysis are listed in Table 2. We also conducted a network analysis by IPA. In the top-ranked networks, these genes play an important role in Behavior (IPA disease and Function Term, P-value = 1.02E-16~1.39E-6), including memory (P-value = 1.39E-6), learning (P-value = 9.11E-9) and behavior (P-value = 1.02E-16) (Table 3). Besides, these genes are also abundant in the network related to Cell Morphology (IPA disease and Function Term, P-value = 5.42E-16~2.26E-03). For DNBs in this network (Fig. 4), i.e., FOS, JUN and EGR1 are regulated by many genes, while SRF and NR3C1 regulate other genes in the network. The FOS gene family consists of FOS, FOSB, FOSL1, and FOSL2. Leucine zipper proteins are encoded by FOS and can dimerize with proteins of the JUN family. FOS and JUN form the transcription factor complex AP-1. These proteins have been identified as regulators of cell proliferation, differentiation, and transformation. The protein encoded by EGR1 belongs to growth factor EGR family and functions as a transcriptional regulator. Its target genes play a role in differentiation and mutagenesis. Upstream analysis by IPA shows that FOS, JUN and EGR1 have the same upstream regulator, growth factor EGF and transcription regulator FOS. SRF encodes a ubiquitous nuclear protein and stimulates cell differentiation and proliferation. NR3C1 encodes glucocorticoid receptor and participates in inflammatory reactions, cell differentiation and proliferation in target tissues [43]. Based on the analyses above, clearly these DNBs are related to Behavior in the functional network, and they have strong co-function links with other genes.
Infants grow at the fastest rate at 3 months old, not only for their height and weight but also for their brain development. The head circumference will increase by about 1.25 cm during the third month. Their behavior undergoes huge changes during this period. The Moro reflex, which is developed in human evolution as a response to a sudden loss of support, starts disappearing gradually at this period [44]. When the soles of infant feet touch a flat surface, they will attempt to walk by placing one foot in front of the other: this phenomenon is called as walking reflex or stepping reflex. This behavior is present at birth and disappears around 10 weeks [45]. At 3 months old, infants’ unconscious activities, such as Moro reflex and Stepping reflect, disappears, meanwhile their behavior develops more purposefully and steadily towards maturation. In addition, during that period, hands begin to perform advanced functions and can open and close consciously. They start staring at something that interests them as well. Their Hand-eye coordination is also improving.
In a brief summary, our human DNBs and DEGs are found abundant in Nervous System Development and Function, Cell Morphology and Behavior. Nervous System Development and Cell Morphology support the fast brain development, so that, infants become to develop the advanced behavior. In the molecular level, we identify several DNBs as the hubs of the network, which play critical roles in regulating the relevant functional modules and pathways.
Chimpanzee acquires the ability of basic behaviors associated with survival at the tipping point
By contrast, chimpanzee develops faster than human, which has been observed in many research works [13]. Actually, in this work we find that chimpanzee reaches the tipping point (1 month old) earlier. We also apply IPA analysis on chimpanzee DNBs and DEGs before and after 1 month old. The most significant physiological system development and function terms from disease and function analysis are listed in Table 4. Though chimpanzee DNBs and DEGs have few overlaps with human DNBs and DEGs, their top physiological system development and function terms are very similar, including nervous system development, organismal development and tissue morphology. We can conclude that chimpanzee experiences the basic brain development similar to human beings at the tipping point. We further investigate the ability of behaviors by the function analysis, i.e., the feeding (P-value = 3.92E-02). Human has more advanced behavior ability at the tipping point than chimpanzee, such as learning and memory, which are the footstone of our cognitive competence.
Discussion and conclusion
Human beings have had dramatic enlargement in brain size during the evolution, and developed advanced cognitive ability. In this work, we aim to reveal major biological events happening to infant human brain in the development process. We applied the DNB theory to analyze the tipping points based on the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex expression data of both human and chimpanzee.
Generally, chimpanzee grows faster than human beings for the brain development; they reach the tipping point around 1 month old, much earlier than human. In contrast, human’s tipping point is around 3 months old, which has been reported as one of the golden age of brain development [12]. Then we carried out the differential gene expression analysis around the tipping points, and conducted function analysis on DEGs and DNBs with IPA. Chimpanzee and human have different DEGs and DNBs, but their DEGs and DNBs are related to same physiological system development functions, i.e., nervous system development, organismal development and tissue morphology. These functions support the fast development of brain at this time period. However, human further obtains more advanced behavior functions at the tipping point. Specifically, human infants have the ability of learning and memory while chimpanzee infants do not. In the molecular level, we identified several DNBs in the hubs of human top ranked network, which play a critical role in regulating the modules and having the functional impacts on the brain growth. They are of great importance due to the related functions with cellular proliferation and differentiation in common. This work not only provides biological insights into the brain development at a molecular level but also opens a new way to study the criticality of nonlinear biological processes based on the observed omics data. As a future topic, in contrast to the traditional correlation analysis, we will adopt the direct associations [12] between molecules to study the molecular mechanism of infant brain development at the network level.
Methods
Data set preparation
All dorsolarteral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) expression data sets from the microarray experiments were downloaded from the National Center for Biotechnology Information Gene Expression Omnibus (GEO) with the accession numbers GSE11512 (GC HG-U133 Plus2.0 experiments). The data sets contain 44 human samples (ranging in age 0–80 years), 14 chimpanzee samples (ranging in age 0–44 years), and 9 macaque samples. In each sampling period, there are 1 to 5 samples for gene expressions (Additional file 5).
Clustering analyses of data sets
Before analyzing the two data sets, we transformed the original data by using normalization method. In this way, the data of each sample is in a uniform distribution which suits better for our statistical analysis. One method of cluster algorithm is ‘clustergram()’ in MATLAB Library, and the results are shown in Fig. 1a. Another one is PCA, and the results are given in Fig. 1b. MDS is used to calculate a 1D summary measure of global expression relative to the individual’s age (Fig. 1c). The ‘pdist()’ function in MATLAB Library is used to evaluate the distance between two samples, and the ‘mdscale()’ in MATLAB Library is adopted to estimate the global expression of samples.
Dynamic network biomarkers (DNB) analysis
Based on the nonlinear dynamical theory, a system is near the critical state if there is a dominant group of molecules, i.e. DNB. According to the DNB theoretical analysis [14], we proved that the following generic properties hold when the infant brain biological system reaches a critical time point.
There exists a group of molecules of human or chimpanzee, whose average Pearson’s correlation coefficients (PCCs) of molecules drastically increase in absolute value.
The average PCCs of molecules between this group and any others (i.e., between molecules inside this group and any other molecules outside this group) drastically decrease in absolute value.
The average standard deviations (SDs) of molecules in this group drastically increase.
If all of these three conditions are satisfied simultaneously, we call this group a dominant group of the system, which will play an important role in early brain development.
Therefore, when a biological system approaches the tipping point; a dominant group of genes appear among all genes. With the gene expression data of all samples in one period, this dominant group can be quantified by the following composite index (DNB model):
where PCCd is the average Pearson’s correlation coefficient (PCC) between the genes in the dominant group of the same time period in absolute value; PCCo is the average PCC between the dominant group and others of the same time period in absolute value; SDd is the average standard deviations (SD) of the genes in the dominant group. These three criteria together construct the composite index (CI) [14, 15, 24,25,26,27,28, 46,47,48,49,50,51]. The CI is expected to reach the peak or increase sharply during the measured periods when the system approaches the tipping point, thus indicating the imminent transition.
We applied this DNB method to detect the tipping points during the infant human and chimpanzee brain development. In each sampling period of infant human and chimpanzee, there are 1–5 samples with gene expression profiles. In order to increase the reliability of DNB result, the slide window method is incorporated into DNB model to process data. We calculate these three criteria of human and chimpanzee (Additional file 2). In addition, the Matlab package of DNB and the operation methods of DNB model are available at http://sysbio.sibcb.ac.cn/cb/chenlab/software.htm
Functional analysis
DNBs and DEGs for the two species are used for pathway enrichment analysis by IPA [42] (Additional file 6), and network analysis is used to investigate the correlations of DNBs and DEGs in IPA network analysis. All additional results are described in Additional file 6 in this paper.
Availability of data and materials
All data generated or analysed during this study are included in this published article and its supplementary information files.
Abbreviations
- DEG:
-
Differentially expressed gene
- DEGs:
-
Differentially expressed genes
- DLPFC:
-
Dorsolarteral prefrontal cortex
- DNB:
-
Dynamic network biomarker
- DNBs:
-
Dynamic network biomarker genes
- MDS:
-
Multi-dimensional scaling
- PCA:
-
Principal component analysis
- PCC:
-
Pearson’s correlation coefficient
- SD:
-
Standard deviations
References
Workman AD, Charvet CJ, Clancy B, Darlington RB, Finlay BL. Modeling transformations of neurodevelopmental sequences across mammalian species. J Neurosci. 2013;33(17):7368–83.
Mikkelsen TS, Hillier LW, Eichler EE, Zody MC, Jaffe DB, Yang SP, Enard W, Hellmann I, Lindblad-Toh K, Altheide TK, et al. Initial sequence of the chimpanzee genome and comparison with the human genome. Nature. 2005;437(7055):69–87.
Szathmary E, Smith JM. The major evolutionary transitions. Nature. 1995;374(6519):227–32.
Carroll SB. Genetics and the making of Homo sapiens. Nature. 2003;422(6934):849–57.
Leigh SR. Brain growth, life history, and cognition in primate and human evolution. Am J Primatol. 2004;62(3):139–64.
Langer J. The Heterochronic Evolution of Primate Cognitive Development. Biol Theory. 2006;(issue 1):3.
Herrmann E, Call J, Hernandez-Lloreda MV, Hare B, Tomasello M. Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science. 2007;317(5843):1360–6.
Pollard KS, Salama SR, King B, Kern AD, Dreszer T, Katzman S, Siepel A, Pedersen JS, Bejerano G, Baertsch R, et al. Forces shaping the fastest evolving regions in the human genome. PLoS Genet. 2006;2(10):e168.
Pollard KS, Salama SR, Lambert N, Lambot MA, Coppens S, Pedersen JS, Katzman S, King B, Onodera C, Siepel A, et al. An RNA gene expressed during cortical development evolved rapidly in humans. Nature. 2006;443(7108):167–72.
Prabhakar S, Noonan JP, Paabo S, Rubin EM. Accelerated evolution of conserved noncoding sequences in humans. Science. 2006;314(5800):786.
Penin X, Berge C, Baylac M. Ontogenetic study of the skull in modern humans and the common chimpanzees: Neotenic hypothesis reconsidered with a tridimensional procrustes analysis. Am J Phys Anthropol. 2002;118(1):50–62.
Holland D, Chang LD, Ernst TM, Curran M, Buchthal SD, Alicata D, Skranes J, Johansen H, Hernandez A, Yamakawa R, et al. Structural growth trajectories and rates of change in the first 3 months of infant brain development. JAMA Neurol. 2014;71(10):1266–74.
Somel M, Franz H, Yan Z, Lorenc A, Guo S, Giger T, Kelso J, Nickel B, Dannemann M, Bahn S, et al. Transcriptional neoteny in the human brain. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2009;106(14):5743–8.
Chen LN, Liu R, Liu ZP, Li MY, Aihara K. Detecting early-warning signals for sudden deterioration of complex diseases by dynamical network biomarkers. Sci Rep Uk. 2012;2.
Liu R, Wang X, Aihara K, Chen L. Early diagnosis of complex diseases by molecular biomarkers, network biomarkers, and dynamical network biomarkers. Med Res Rev. 2014;34(3):455–78.
Scheffer M, Carpenter S, Foley JA, Folke C, Walker B. Catastrophic shifts in ecosystems. Nature. 2001;413(6856):591–6.
Drake JM, Griffen BD. Early warning signals of extinction in deteriorating environments. Nature. 2010;467(7314):456–9.
Lenton TM, Held H, Kriegler E, Hall JW, Lucht W, Rahmstorf S, Schellnhuber HJ. Tipping elements in the Earth’s climate system. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(6):1786–93.
Dakos V, Scheffer M, van Nes EH, Brovkin V, Petoukhov V, Held H. Slowing down as an early warning signal for abrupt climate change. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(38):14308–12.
May RM, Levin SA, Sugihara G. Complex systems - ecology for bankers. Nature. 2008;451(7181):893–5.
Kambhu J, Weidman S, Krishnan N. New Directions for Understanding Systemic Risk: A Report on a Conference Cosponsored by the Federal Reserve Bank of New York and the National Academy of Sciences. Econ Policy Rev. 2007.
Li MY, Zeng T, Liu R, Chen LN. Detecting tissue-specific early warning signals for complex diseases based on dynamical network biomarkers: study of type 2 diabetes by cross-tissue analysis. Brief Bioinform. 2014;15(2):229–43.
Zeng T, Zhang CC, Zhang WW, Liu R, Liu J, Chen LN. Deciphering early development of complex diseases by progressive module network. Methods. 2014;67(3):334–43.
Liu XP, Chang X, Liu R, Yu XT, Chen LN, Aihara K. Quantifying critical states of complex diseases using single-sample dynamic network biomarkers. Plos Comput Biol. 2017;13(7).
Liu R, Wang J, Ukai M, Sewon K, Chen P, Suzuki Y, Wang H, Aihara K, Okada-Hatakeyama M, Chen L. Hunt for the tipping point during endocrine resistance process in breast cancer by dynamic network biomarkers. J Mol Cell Biol. 2019;11(8):649–64.
Yang BW, Li MY, Tang WQ, Liu WX, Zhang S, Chen LN, Xia JL. Dynamic network biomarker indicates pulmonary metastasis at the tipping point of hepatocellular carcinoma. Nat Commun. 2018;9.
Li MY, Li C, Liu WX, Liu CH, Cui JR, Li QR, Ni H, Yang YC, Wu CC, Chen CL, et al. Dysfunction of PLA2G6 and CYP2C44-associated network signals imminent carcinogenesis from chronic inflammation to hepatocellular carcinoma. J Mol Cell Biol. 2017;9(6):489–503.
Richard A, Boullu L, Herbach U, Bonnafoux A, Morin V, Vallin E, Guillemin A, Gao NP, Gunawan R, Cosette J, et al. Single-Cell-Based Analysis Highlights a Surge in Cell-to-Cell Molecular Variability Preceding Irreversible Commitment in a Differentiation Process. Plos Biol. 2016;14(12).
Lesterhuis WJ, Bosco A, Millward MJ, Small M, Nowak AK, Lake RA. Dynamic versus static biomarkers in cancer immune checkpoint blockade: unravelling complexity. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2017;16(4):264–72.
Jung H, Hsiung B, Pestal K, Procyk E, Raulet DH. RAE-1 ligands for the NKG2D receptor are regulated by E2F transcription factors, which control cell cycle entry. J Exp Med. 2012;209(13):2409–22.
Rogers KW, Lord ND, Gagnon JA, Pauli A, Zimmermann S, Aksel DC, Reyon D, Tsai SDQ, Joung JK, Schier AF. Nodal patterning without lefty inhibitory feedback is functional but fragile. Elife. 2017;6.
Stottmann RW, Donlin M, Hafner A, Bernard A, Sinclair DA, Beier DR. A mutation in Tubb2b, a human polymicrogyria gene, leads to lethality and abnormal cortical development in the mouse. Hum Mol Genet. 2013;22(20):4053–63.
Ambjorn M, Dubreuil V, Miozzo F, Nigon F, Moller B, Issazadeh-Navikas S, Berg J, Lees M, Sap J. A Loss-of-Function Screen for Phosphatases that Regulate Neurite Outgrowth Identifies PTPN12 as a Negative Regulator of TrkB Tyrosine Phosphorylation. Plos One. 2013;8(6).
Yue W, Yu X, Zhang D. Progress in genome-wide association studies of schizophrenia in Han Chinese populations. NPJ Schizophr. 2017;3(1):24.
Mariani E, Frabetti F, Tarozzi A, Pelleri MC, Pizzetti F, Casadei R. Meta-Analysis of Parkinson's Disease Transcriptome Data Using TRAM Software: Whole Substantia Nigra Tissue and Single Dopamine Neuron Differential Gene Expression. Plos One. 2016;11(9).
Gialluisi A, Visconti A, Willcutt EG, Smith SD, Pennington BF, Falchi M, DeFries JC, Olson RK, Francks C, Fisher SE. Investigating the effects of copy number variants on reading and language performance. J Neurodev Disord. 2016;8.
Snapkov I, Oqvist CO, Figenschau YA, Kogner P, Johnsen JI, Sveinbjornsson B. The role of formyl peptide receptor 1 (FPR1) in neuroblastoma tumorigenesis. Cancer Res. 2015;75.
Kasukawa T, Masumoto K, Nikaido I, Nagano M, Uno KD, Tsujino K, Hanashima C, Shigeyoshi Y, Ueda HR. Quantitative Expression Profile of Distinct Functional Regions in the Adult Mouse Brain. Plos One. 2011;6(8).
Glass LJ, Sinclair D, Boerrigter D, Naude K, Fung SJ, Brown D, Catts VS, Tooney P, O'Donnell M, Lenroot R, et al. Brain antibodies in the cortex and blood of people with schizophrenia and controls. Transl Psychiat. 2017;7.
Zhu CB, Kros JM, van der Weiden M, Zheng PP, Cheng C, Mustafa DAM. Expression site of P2RY12 in residential microglial cells in astrocytomas correlates with M1 and M2 marker expression and tumor grade. Acta Neuropathol Com. 2017:5.
Xie C, Ginet V, Sun Y, Koike M, Zhou K, Li T, Li H, Li Q, Wang X, Uchiyama Y, et al. Neuroprotection by selective neuronal deletion of Atg7 in neonatal brain injury. Autophagy. 2016;12(2):410–23.
Ingenuity Pathway Analysis (IPA) (http://www.ingenuity.com). Accessed 10 Aug 2018.
Lu NZ, Cidlowski JA. Translational regulatory mechanisms generate N-terminal glucocorticoid receptor isoforms with unique transcriptional target genes. Mol Cell. 2005;18(3):331–42.
MD MAF: Physical diagnosis in Neonotology. Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven; 1997.
Siegler S. R: How Children Develop. New York: Worth Publishers; 2010.
Liu R, Yu XT, Liu XP, Xu D, Aihara K, Chen LN. Identifying critical transitions of complex diseases based on a single sample. Bioinformatics. 2014;30(11):1579–86.
Zhang XJ, Liu KQ, Liu ZP, Duval B, Richer JM, Zhao XM, Hao JK, Chen LN. NARROMI: a noise and redundancy reduction technique improves accuracy of gene regulatory network inference. Bioinformatics. 2013;29(1):106–13.
Zhao J, Zhou YW, Zhang XJ, Chen LN. Part mutual information for quantifying direct associations in networks. P Natl Acad Sci USA. 2016;113(18):5130–5.
Zhang XJ, Zhao J, Hao JK, Zhao XM, Chen LN. Conditional mutual inclusive information enables accurate quantification of associations in gene regulatory networks. Nucleic Acids Res. 2015;43(5).
Liu R, Wang J, Ukai M, Sewon K, Chen P, Suzuki Y, Wang H, Aihara K, Okada-Hatakeyama M, Chen L. Hunt for the tipping point during endocrine resistance process in breast cancer by dynamic network biomarkers. J Mol Cell Biol. 2018.
Liu X, Chang X, Leng S, Tang H, Aihara K, Chen L. Detection for disease tipping points by landscape dynamic network biomarkers. Natl Sci Rev. 2018.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank all members of the laboratory for valuable discussions.
About this supplement
This article has been published as part of BMC Genomics, Volume 21 Supplement 1, 2020: Selected articles from the 14th International Symposium on Bioinformatics Research and Applications (ISBRA-18): genomics. The full contents of the supplement are available at https://bmcgenomics.biomedcentral.com/articles/supplements/volume-21-supplement-1.
Funding
This work was supported by the National key research and development program of China (No. 2017YFA0505500), by Shanghai Municipal Science and Technology Major Project (No. 2017SHZDZX01), the National Natural Science Foundation of China (NSFC) (Nos. 61403363, 11401222, 11871456, 31200987, 31930022, 31771476), and the Natural Science Foundation of Shanghai (17ZR1446100). Publication costs are funded by the National key research and development program of China (No. 2017YFA0505500).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
HT and LNC developed the methodology. HT and YT executed the experiment and do the data analysis, HT, YT and TZ wrote this paper. HT, TZ and LNC revised the manuscript. LNC supervised the work, made critical revisions of the paper, and approved the submission of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
Not applicable.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
Additional file 1.
Hierarchical cluster analysis of normalized DPLEC datasets (Human, Chimpanzee, Monkey), based on 17,429 expressed genes. The red represents human, the blue represents chimpanzee and the yellow represents macaque.
Additional file 2.
Detecting the tipping points for the infant brain development of human and chimpanzee. Detecting the tipping points for two data sets, human (a, b, c, d) and chimpanzee (e, f, g, h). The infant human contain 13 samples (age range from 0 to 0.9 years old). The infant chimpanzee contains 9 samples (age range from 0 to 6.3 years old). Subfigures a and e represent the composite index (see Methods, CI in Eq.(1)), Subfigures b and f represent the mean SDs in the DNB of human and chimpanzee (see Methods, SD in Eq.(1)), Subfigures c and g represent PCCs in the DNB (see Methods, PCCd in Eq.(1)), Subfigures d and h are the PCCs between the DNB and other molecules (see Methods, PCCo in Eq.(1)).The results of the figure show the effectiveness of the DNB model by using our data sets and 3 and 1 month are the tipping points of two species.
Additional file 3.
371 DNBs of human and 132 DNBs of chimpanzee. There are one gene which are overlaps of two parts DNBs.
Additional file 4.
The DEGs of human and chimpanzee around the tipping point. The overlap genes between DNBs and DEGs of two species.
Additional file 5.
Sample Characteristics.
Additional file 6.
Detailed DNB genes and DEGs IPA analysis summary and network analysis results.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Tang, H., Tang, Y., Zeng, T. et al. Gene expression analysis reveals the tipping points during infant brain development for human and chimpanzee. BMC Genomics 21 (Suppl 1), 74 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12864-020-6465-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12864-020-6465-8